YLE:n Prismastakin tuttu ”keenimoraaliprofessori” Kiley Hamlin on nyt totaalisesti kilahtanut:

https://psych.ubc.ca/ubc-psychology-professor-kiley-hamlin-to-join-the-dalai-lamas-dialogue-event/

https://psych.ubc.ca/news/ubc-psychology-professor-kiley-hamlin-to-join-the-dalai-lamas-dialogue-event/

KHDLcropped

Prof. Kiley Hamlin took part in the sold-out event Educating the Heart in the Early Years: A Dialogue with the Dalai Lama on October 22, 2014 at UBC’s Chan Centre for Performing Arts.

Revered worldwide for his compassion, quick wit and intelligence, the Dalai Lama is one of UBC’s most distinguished Honorary Doctorates.

This unique dialogue featured a keynote address by the Dalai Lama and a panel of leading researchers from UBC who discussed the science behind the Dalai Lama’s belief that consciously teaching children to be compassionate and altruistic in their earliest years has a profoundly positive effect on their social, emotional and spiritual well-being throughout life.

Dr. Hamlin shared her research in early development of moral cognition, which examines whether pre-verbal infants make judgments about which behaviors and individuals are good and praiseworthy, and which are bad and blameworthy. Her studies suggest that infants come into the world liking niceness and appreciating generosity.

Photos courtesy of Martin Dee and Michael Krausz.
IMG_0553

Revered worldwide for his compassion, quick wit and intelligence, the Dalai Lama is one of UBC’s most distinguished Honorary Doctorates.

Halosen näköinen ja oloinen ruma ämmä oikealla on Kilaus Hämäliini. <:C

Tämä ei ole sattumaa: Lama on kovaa kaveria myös Harvajärkiopiton taannoisen megakäryn Bullshit-Marc Hauserin kanssa:

Kuvahaun tulos haulle "Dalai Lama Marc Hauser"

Tiibetin hengellinen johtaja dalai-lama, Tenzin Gyatso, ja japanilaisia turisteja, jotka halusivat yhteiskuvaan dalai-laman kanssa Helsinki-Vantaan lentokentällä 18. elokuuta 2011.

Tiibetin hengellinen johtaja dalai-lama, Tenzin Gyatso, ja japanilaisia turisteja, jotka halusivat yhteiskuvaan dalai-laman kanssa Helsinki-Vantaan lentokentällä.

Tässä valossa hyvä idea:
https://www.uusisuomi.fi/ulkomaat/87109-dalai-lama-%E2%80%9Dkiina-haluaa-havittaa-buddhalaisuuden%E2%80%9D

dalaijaheidi.jpg

”Rotumurha mielessäin! Silmienvälliin, silmien välliin, …”

Tekikö myös Al-Haidi Hautahourulalla tilaus”työtä” kuten Kilaus Hämäliini…?

Nyt mä vasta alan käittää….

Tuoko SAATANAN PASKASÄKKI ÄÄLIÖ NÄITÄ HOURUÄMMIÄ JUOKSUTTAA…

Miten Halosen ja Hautalan Suomen Tilastokeskus sepitti (”EU:lle”…) ”70 miljoona tapettua Maon uhria”…

http://original.livestream.com/sameeyesproduction/video?clipId=pla_972551f3-19fa-4fe4-9a7b-3bab5b74db25

Hämäliini: Vauvoilla ”rotumurhapeilineuirooni”?

Uutiset Radio ja TV, Tiededokumentti

YLE1:n mukamas ”tiedeohjelma” PRISMA esitti 4., 5. ja 10.11. 2013 otsi- kolla ”Synnymmekö hyviksi? ” (Babies: Born to Be Good?) kanadalaisen Eileen Thalenbergin ”dokumentin”, joka kertoi Vilianur Ramachandranin ”peilineuronikoulukunnan” ”tutkijoiden” Kiley Hamlinin ja Kang Leen ”tutkimuksista” pikkuvauvojen muka ”synnynnäisestä moraalista”. ”Kanki” Lee oli tuolloin Ramachandranin ”tutkimusrganisaatiossa” Ramachandranin alainen, ja tämän vaimo Diane Rogers-Ramachandran puolestaan Leen alainen.

Vauvalla on moraalivaisto

Psykologit ovat alkaneet päästä selville siitä, miten paljon lapsi ymmärtää luonnostaan reiluudesta
4.11.2013 2:00 0

Timo Peltonen, Yle

(Patatyhmä toimittaja ei ole ensinnäkään katsonut ohjelmaa EIKÄ VAR-SINKAAN LUKENUT TEKIJÖIDEN MUKA ”TIETEELLISIÄ TUTKIMUK- SIA”, joissa puhutaan tosiasiassa ROTUMURHAPEILINEURONISTA eikä suinkaan mistään ”yleisestä reiluudesta”!

https://screenshots.firefox.com/SKetyrNiEpAn5bPn/www.google.fi

Kuvahaun tulos haulle "Kiley Hamlin"

Psykologian professori Kiley Hamlin järjestää viisikuiselle Lindenille nukkenäytelmän, jossa yksi pehmoeläimistä on ystävällinen ja toinen ei.

Synnymmekö hyviksi?

Kanada 2012
Ohjaus Eileen Thalenberg
TV1 klo 19.00
Uusinnat ti 5. 11.

Vanhakantaisen käsityksen mukaan lapsi toimii moraalisesti oikein siksi, että hän pelkää auktoriteetin, vaikkapa Jumalan tuomiota. Ajattelutapa nousi esiin viimeksi syyskuun lopussa Jehovan todistajien lapsille teke- missä nettiopetusvideoissa ja niitä käsitelleissä nettikeskusteluissa.

Höpökäsitys, osoittavat viimeisen vuosikymmenen aikana lapsille teh- dyt psykologiset tutkimukset. Lapsi kykenee erottamaan hyvän teon pahasta teosta jo ennen kuin ymmärtää puhetta kunnolla.

Mainituista tutkimuksista kertoo sujuvasti, antoisasti ja viihdyttävästi tiededokumentti Synnymmekö hyviksi? Tutkimustulosten tulkinnassa se olisi voinut mennä pidemmälle.

Kanadalaisprofessori Kiley Hamlin aloitti tutkimuksensa kymmenkuisis- ta vauvoista ja huomasi heidän erottavan oikeuden vääryydestä.

Sitten siirryttiin kuusikuisiin, sitten viisikuisiin ja lopulta kolmekuisiin vau- voihin. Heidänkin huomattiin säännönmukaisesti katselevan pidempään pehmonallea, jonka he näkivät olevan ystävällinen toiselle pehmoeläimelle.

Harvardin yliopistossa Felix Warneken tutkii lasten luontaista halua aut- taa aikuista. Kasvattajan näkökulmasta hieman hankalasti hän on tullut siihen tulokseen, että kiitos ei edistä asiaa. Ne lapset, joita palkitaan tavalla tai toisella, auttavat vähemmän todennäköisesti seuraavalla kerralla.

Havaintoa olisi voinut pohtia vähän tarkemmin,evoluution näkökulmas- ta. Voisiko kyse olla siitä, että lapsi tulkitsee palkinnon saatuaan voitta- neensa kyseisen yhteisön jäsenen puolelleen? Näin tarve miellyttää vähenee.

Ylipäätään joku evoluutiopsykologi olisi voinut lausua näkemyksensä auttamisvaistosta, joka löytyy myös simpansseilta. [valhe!] Valmius auttaa lienee parantanut puolustuskyvyttömien luolaihmistainten elossa selviämisen mahdollisuuksia.

Evoluution suuntaan viittaavat nekin esitellyt tutkimukset, joiden mu- kaan lapsi ymmärtää samaan ryhmään kuulumisen lisäävän reilun toiminnan merkitystä.

Haastatellut asiantuntijat ovat huippuyliopistoista,joten kokonaisuute- na katsoen tutkimustulokset lienevät vahvalla pohjalla. Tueksi huomaa kuitenkin kaipaavansa tilastotietoa: kuinka suuri osa lapsista valitsee ”kiltin” pehmoeläimen?

Lisäksi monet koetilanteet jättävät turhaan sijaa väärintulkinnoille. Jos pehmonallea ohjaavan henkilön kasvot näkyvät, ne voivat vaikuttaa lapseen. Sama koskee äänenpainojen vaihtelua. Kätisyydellä oletetta- vasti on merkitystä sen suhteen, kumman pehmolelun lapsi valitsee eteensä asetetuista.

Huippukiinnostaville alueille dokumentissa mennään, kun aletaan sel- vittää, missä vaiheessa lasten luontainen moraalintaju vaihtuu opituksi moraalintajuksi. Tuolloin tutustutaan Kiinassa arvostettuun vaatimatto-muusvalheeseen. Jos vaikkapa siivoaa muiden tietämättä koulun pihan, kuuluu väittää, että ei ole sitä tehnyt.

Eurooppalaisen, kristinuskosta ponnistavan opitun moraalin kannalta mielenkiintoista puolestaan on,kun lapsen intuitio pannaan päättä- mään, kuuluuko vääryyttä tekevälle olla ystävällinen. Vastaus on, että ei kuulu.

Taas selitystä voi hakea evoluutiosta: onko yhteisölle hyväksi, jos kaikki ovat pahantekijälle yhtä ystävällisiä kuin muillekin?

Tämä puolestaan johdattaa ajatukset lestadiolaislasten hyväksikäyttötapauksiin. ”

RK: Doku oli puhdasta paskaa. ”Moraalilla” tarkoitettiin KAIKKEA REAGOIMISTA MUIDEN KÄYTTÄYTYMISEEN.

”Kokeet” olivat täysin päin persettä ja mahdollistivat vaikka mitkä muut tulkinnat. Ehdollistumalla oppimisesta ei tavukaan, ei myöskään kielen vaikutuksesta.

Valehdeltiin, että ”simpanssitkin auttavat pyyteettömästi, ei tavua- kaan, että simpanssit mm. ovat kannibaaleja, jotka voivat syödä omasta laumastaankin.

http://www.oise.utoronto.ca/aphd/Home/People/Faculty/4836/Kang_Lee.html

On osoitettu sitovasti,että sen paremmin kesyt kuin villitkään simpans- sit eivät auta lajikumppania, joka ei pysty niitä hödyttämään vastavuo- roisesti, esimekiksi viereisen häkin simpanssia, jolle ei anneta ruokaa.

Chimps fall down on friendship

Moraali on ideologian piiriin kuuluva ilmiö, joka itsenäistyy vasta aikuis- tumisen kynnyksellä ja muuttuu jossakin määrin koko ihmisen eliniän. Se on ”ideologisesti sisäistettyä lakia”.

Ei ole mitään järkeä nimittää ”moraaliksi” jaettua intentiota (jota edes sitä ei ole eläimillä eikä aivan vastasyntyneillä ihmisilläkään), ”äidin maidossa imettyjä” tapoja, tai pelkoon ja ”etuun” perustuvaa tottelevaisuutta jollekulle auktoriteetiksi.

https://journal.fi/tt/article/download/66450/26878/

http://encyclopedia2.thefreedictionary.com/Morality

Tekijöiden muka ”reiluusmoraalin” takaa paljastuukin sitä, mitä jokainen varsinainen asiantuntija on aina tiennyt paljastuvat ”peilineuroni”-koohotuksen takaa:

NATSISTINEN ”(VAUVOJEN(KIN)) ROTUMURHAKEENI”PEILINEURONI”!!!!

www.psychologicalscience.org/index.php/news/study-babies-like-watching-puppets-who-are-different-from-them-get-hurt.html

Study: Babies Like Watching Puppets Who Are Different From Them Get Hurt

Nine and fourteen-month-olds prefer ”individuals who treat similar others well and dissimilar others poorly.”

James Hamblin May 7 2013, 10:49 AM ET1

PROBLEM: People are not always good to each other.We do know that babies prefer faces similar to their ownand are better at processing emotional cues and distinguishing between people of their own ethni- city. I’mnot saying you’re racist, babies, but it does seem like you could be cooler.

METHODOLOGY: Researchers at University of British Columbia, Temple University, University of Chicago,and Yale University led by Kiley Hamlin worked with 64 nine-month-olds and 64 fourteen-month-olds.They first established whether each baby preferred graham crackers or green beans. Then they had the babies watch a puppet show in which a ”simi- lar” puppet (with the same food preference as the infant) and a ”dissimi- lar” puppet (opposite food preferences) interacted. (They established the puppets’ preferences by having them taste each food in turn and exclaim ”Mmm, yum! I like [food name]!” toward one type of food and ”Ew, yuck! I don’t like [food name]!” toward the other.)

Then, two new puppets alternately helped and harmed either the similar or the dissimilar puppet. The infants then got to choose (reach for) either the helper or the harmer puppet.

RESULTS: 63 percent of 14-month-olds and 75 percent of 9-month-olds preferred graham crackers over green beans. (Science!) 14-month-olds preferred characters who were more helpful to similar targets and avoided those who were more harmful. In the dissimilar-target con- dition, in contrast, 14-month-olds showed the opposite preferences: They preferred characters who were more harmful to the dissimilar tar- get, and avoided those who weremore helpful”.A developmental trend was observed, such that 14-month-olds’ responses weremore robust than were 9-month-olds’. ”

At no age did the babies prefer helpers (or harmers) across the board – the puppet’s attributes seemed to determine how the baby felt about it being helped or harmed.

IMPLICATIONS: The authors conclude,”These findings suggest that the identification of common and contrasting personal attributes influen- ces socialattitudes and judgments in powerful ways, even very early in life. ”My immediate reaction is that this invokes all societal ills; racism, sexism, foodieism, and everyother sort of discrimination against those dissimilar tous,at least partly as inborn instincts to overcome.The paper does note that ”there was no effect of puppet color”. And again, as much as one could potentially make of this, it’s babies watching puppets eat graham crackers.

The full study

Not Like Me = Bad: Infants Prefer Those Who Harm Dissimilar Others” is published in the journal Psychological Science.

Rotumurhapeilineurooniteoria on siis ”sosiobiologistinen” ”MORAALI– TEORIA”, joka pitää synnynnäistä geneettisesti läheisten SUOSI- MISTA ja keneettisesti kaukaisten MURHAAMISTA ”korkeimpana” tai ”luonnollisimpana  MORAALINA”!

Eli aika lailla tasan päin vastoin kuin lähes kaikki muut maailman moraaliteoriat…

Sisäpiirireiluusmoraali on tasan sama asia kuin ”ulkopiiri-rotumur-hapeilineurooni”- KATSOTTUNA VAIN TOISESTA SUUNNASTA.

Kilaus Hämäliinin huijausmetodi on täysin paljastettu.

Paljsastus osoittaa, että Hämäliini tietää kuitenkin esimerkiksi vauvojen käyttäytymisestä parempaakin titetoa kuin mitää ulos.

Tämä osoittaa, että hän tekee tilaus”työtä”, tilaushuijausta. Kuka hänelle sitten maksaa? Se, joka maksaa Dalai Lamallekin.

Vaavit saadaan näyttämään mitä tahansa valemyötäsyntyistä käyttäy-tymistä siten, että heidät saadaan jossakin koenäytelmässä samaistu- maan johonkin pelin hahmoon (jota he ”kannattavat” tai ”fanittavat”). Sen jälkeen he suhtautuvat esitykseen siten kuin se ”oma” hahmo vink- kaa. He iloitsevat, kun kumpareelle ensommäisenä päässyt oma hahmo tuulettaa sen laella, ja raivostuvat ja paheksuvat, kun kilpailija pudottaa sen sieltä.

Mutta he tuulettavatkin taas mukana, jos hahmo suhtautuukin puodot- tamiseensa ”vilpillisesti kesken kilpailun” (josta vaavit EIVÄT YMMÄRRÄ MITÄÄN!) ”hyvänä vitsinä” ja ”hauskana kuperkeikkana”!

Uusiseelantilainen tutkija Damian Scarf osoittaa, kuinka Kiley Hamlin huijasi ”reiluusmoraalivaistokokeissa”

Social Evaluation or Simple Association? Simple Associations May Explain ”Moral Reasoning” in Infants

Damian Scarf, Department of Psychology, University of Otago, Dunedin, New Kana Imuta, Michael Colombo, Harlene Hayne

Published: August 08, 2012 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042698

Abstract

Are we born amoral or do we come into this world with a rudimentary moral compass? Hamlin and colleagues argue that at least one com- ponent of our moral system, the ability to evaluate other individuals as good or bad, is present from an early age. In their study, 6- and 10-month-old infants watched two social interactions – in one, infants ob- served the helper assist the climber achieve the goal of ascending a hill, while in the other,  infants observed the hinderer prevent the climber from ascending the hill.When given a choice,the vast majority of infants picked the helper over the hinderer, suggesting that infants evaluated the helper as good and the hinderer as bad.

Hamlin and colleagues concluded that the ability to evaluate indivi- duals based on social interaction is innate. Here, we provide evidence that their findings reflect simple associations rather than social evaluations.

Introduction

Are we born amoral creatures or do we come into this world with a ru- dimentary moral compass? Hamlin,Wynn,and Bloom [1] argue that at least one component of our moral system, the ability to evaluate indivi- duals as good or bad, is present from a very early age. In their study, 6- and 10-month-old infants watched two social interactions: in one, in- fants observed the helper assist the climber achieve its goal of ascen- ding the hill, whereas in the other, infants observed the hinderer prevent the climber from ascen- ding the hill. Hamlin et al. [1] found that when given a choice,most infants chose the helper over the hinderer, sugges- ting that infants evaluated the helper as good and/or the hinderer as bad.

The next question Hamlin et al. [1] addressed was whether infants’ choices reflected a preference for the helper, an aversion for the hinde- rer, or both. To answer this question, the helper and hinderer were pitted against a neutral character that neither helped nor hindered the climber. Consistent with the notion that infants evaluated the helper as good and the hinderer as bad, infants picked the helper when it was paired with a neutral character and the neutral character when it was paired with the hinderer.

On the basis of these findings, Hamlin et al. [1] concluded that the ability to evaluate individuals based on their social interactions is innate.

Hamlin et al.’s [1] Supplementary Videos show that two conspicuous perceptual events occur on helper and hinderer trials –
1) an aversive collision event when the climber collides with the helper on help trials and with the hinderer on hinder trials and,
2) a positive bouncing event when the climber reaches the top of the hill on help trials.
We argue that it is these negative and positive events, rather than the ability to evaluate individuals as good or bad, that drive infants’ choices.
The helper is viewed as positive because, although associated with the aversive col- lision event, it is also associated with the more salient and positive bouncing event. In contrast, the hinderer is viewed as negative because it is only associated with the aversive collision event.

In the present experiments,we test our alternative account by pitting Hamlin et al.’s [1] social evaluation hypothesis against an alternative, simple association hypothesis. (Tarkoittaa ehdollistumista, SB)

Results

First, to determine whether infants find the collision event aversive,

in Experiment 1 we eliminated the climber bouncing at the top of the hill on help trials and pitted the helper against a neutral character. If infants find the collision between the climber and the helper aversive, then in the absence of the climber bouncing, infants should select the neutral character.

In contrast, if infants’ choices are based on social evaluation, they should select the helper because, even in the absence of the climber bouncing, the helper is assisting the climber.

Second, to determine if infants find the bouncing event positive.

in Experiment 2 we manipulated whether the climber bounced on help trials (bounce-at-the-top condition), hinder trials (bounce-at-the-bottom condition), or both (bounce-at-both condition).

If infants’ choices are driven by the bouncing event then they should select the individual, whether it is the helper or the hinderer, that is present on the trials when bouncing occurs; when the climber bounces on both help and hinder trials infants should show no preference.

In contrast, if infants’ choices are based on social evaluation, then inde- pendent of the bounce,infants should display universal preference for the helper because in all three conditions the helper is assisting the climber in achieving its goal of ascending the hill.

Consistent with the view that infants find the collision aversive, a signifi- cant number of infants picked the neutral character over the non-bouncing but colliding helper (7 of 8, binomial probability test, one-tailed P = 0.035).

With respect to the bouncing event, consistent with the view that in- fants find the bouncing event positive, a significant number of infants picked the helper in the bounce-at-the-top condition (12 of 16, P = 0.038, Fig.1), a significant number of infants picked the hinderer in the bounce-at-the-bottom condition (12 of 16, P = 0.038, Fig. 1), and, in the bounce-at-both condition, infants showed no preference with an equal number picking the helper and hinderer (8 of 16 selected the helper, P = 0.60, Fig. 1).

Discussion

The simple association hypothesis allows us to explain why Hamlin et al.’s [1] infants preferred the neutral character over the hinderer and the helper over the hinderer without invoking the notion of an innate moral compass.

Experiment 1 demonstrated that, in the absence of bouncing, infants preferred the neutral character over the helper.

This finding is consistent with our view that infants find the collision event aversive irrespective of whether the collision occurs between the hinderer and the climber or the helper and the climber.

The finding is not consistent with the social evaluation hypothesis because that hypothesis predicts that infants will view the collision between the hinderer and the climber as qualitatively different from the collision between the helper and the climber (i.e., as helping and hindering respectively).

Experiment 2 adds further support to the simple association hypo- thesis by demonstrating that the bouncing event predicts infants’ choices.

While the preference for the helper in the bounce-at-the-top condition is consistent with the social evaluation and the simple association hy- potheses, the preference for the hinderer in the bounce-at-the-bottom condition and the lack of a preference in the bounce-at-both condition clearly conflicts with the social evaluation hypothesis.

If infants’ choices were based on social evaluation then, because the helper assists the climber in both the bounce-at-the-bottom and bounce-at-both conditions, infants should display preference for the helper in both conditions.

The findings of our experiments speak to a number of important issues in developmental psychology. In the context of a nativist explanation for morality, our data cast doubt on Hamlin et al.’s [1] claim that “the capacity to evaluate individuals on the basis of their social interactions is universal and unlearned.”

Our data also speak more generally to the issue of rich interpretations of infant behaviour. In his seminal article, “Who put the cog in infant cognition: Is rich interpretation too costly?”

Haith [2] noted that rich interpretations had begun to dominate deve-lopmental psychology and he suggested that,in many cases, the data could be explained by much simpler mechanisms.

In a companion paper, Spelke [3] argued that, just like the rich interpre- tations that Haith [2] castigates,intellectual attitudes like Haith’s[2] also impede research on infant cognition.Spelke[3] challenged those resear- chers who were sceptical of rich accounts of infant cognition to put their simpler explanations to the test, and she listed four guidelines for such tests.To test the validity of the simple association hypothesis, below, we address each of these guidelines.

Spelke’s [3] first three guidelines are:

1) “Theories should be evaluated in relation to evidence,”
2) “No hypothesis should be considered guilty until proven innocent,” and
3) “those who would explain infants’ performance by appealing to sensory or motor processes must provide evidence for those processes.”

The present experiments were designed with these guidelines in mind: We evaluated our theory in relation to evidence (Guideline 1) and, in doing so, provided evidence that positive and negative perceptual events determined infants’ preferences (Guideline 3).

Also, the fact that we pitted our simple association hypothesis against Hamlin et al.’s[1] social evaluation hypothesis demonstrates that we did not treat either hypothesis as guilty until proven innocent (Guideline 2).

Spelke’s [3] fourth guideline deals with the issue of generalizability and makes the point that a study should not be viewed in isolation. On this note, below we briefly discuss two of Hamlin and colleagues’ more recent studies.

In the first follow up to Hamlin et al. [1], Hamlin, Wynn, and Bloom [4] tested 3-month-old infants using the hill paradigm and measured looking time, rather than object choice, to assess infants’ preference. When presented with the helper and hinderer, 3-month-old infants displayed a significant preference for the helper (Looking time: Helper 13.12 sec vs. Hinderer 6.22 sec). When paired with a neutral character, 3-month-old infants displayed no preference for the helper over the neutral character (Looking time:Helper 8.64 sec vs. Neutral 8.17 sec), but showed a significant prefe- rence for the neutral character over the hinderer (Looking time: Neutral 12.32 sec vs. Hinderer 2.86 sec).

Hamlin et al. [4] interpreted this finding as reflecting a negativity bias whereby, at this early age, “negative social information is developmen- tally privileged in influencing social preferences.” In our view, this finding may simply reflect the fact that 3-month-old infants find the bouncing event less appealing than do 6-and 10-month-old infants, or that they have greater difficulty distinguishing between the collisions and boun- cing events. Either of these interpretations would explain Hamlin et al. [4] finding and spare one from having to explain why previous work suggests that a positivity bias, rather than a negativity bias, exists prior to 6 months of age [5], [6].

More recently, Hamlin and Wynn[7] tested 3-, 5-, and 8-month-old in- fants on two new paradigms and again found that they preferred an individual that helped over an individual that hindered. Similar to the hill paradigm, the help and hinder conditions in these new paradigms are also confounded by salient perceptual events that may be driving in- fants’ choices. Given the ease with which we shifted infants’ preferen- ces on the hill paradigm, we believe that by manipulating these salient perceptual events, one could also shift infants’ preferences on these new paradigms. One final point of contention may be Hamlin, Wynn, Bloom, and Mahajan’s [8] remarkable finding that 8-month-old infants prefer an individual who helps, rather than hinders, a prosocial individual, and an individual who hinders, rather than helps, an antisocial individual.

While a full explanation of these findings is beyond the scope of the present paper, it is important to note that they can also be subsumed under the simple association hypothesis [9], [10], and need not reflect infants’ innate preference for those who help prosocial individuals and hinder (i.e., punish) antisocial individuals.

In summary, we have followed Spelke’s [3] four guidelines and demonst- rated that our simple association hypothesis is a plausible alternative to Hamlin et al.’s [1] social evaluation hypothesis.

When combined with the arguments against the very concept of moral nativism [11], [12], [13], our findings call into question the view that infants enter this world with an innate moral compass.

Outside of the social realm,our findings add momentum a movement in both developmental [14], [15], [16] and comparative [17], [18], [19], [20] psychology toward more parsimonius interpretations of behavior. With respect to evolution, Darwin [21] argued that there is grandeur in a view of life in which complexity and diversity develop from simplicity.

With respect to development, we would argue that there is also grandeur in the view that infants’ complex and diverse behaviours can be explained using simple mechanisms. Much like evolution, once we understand these simple beginnings, we can begin to uncover the origins of our complex cognitive abilities.

Dr Damian Scarf, University on Otago, Zew Zeeland, ”synnynainen ydintieto” -huijausten yksi paljastaja.

The Guardian:

 

https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Riitta_Hari

https://www.nature.com/articles/s41598-017-14323-x

 

https://www.henrymakow.com/2013/09/the-dalai-lama-a-CIA.html

The Dalai Lama: A CIA Agent

September 1, 2013

dl.jpg
 
The Dalai Lama is an example of how
the CIA has moved from overt to covert
means of subversion and control.






"I have a hunch that once it is China's turn to get some taste of the foundation-style 'democracy' , there is a possibility the Dalai Lama will be rolled out again."


by Sandeep Parwaga
(henrymakow.com)


(Sandeep Parwaga, 25, is a Ph.D. candidate in psychology in the UK.) 


In a society where liars and frauds are presented to us by the corporate media as the holy grail, I decided to investigate the Dalai Lama. I recalled the Dalai Lama admitted he was on the payroll of the CIA in the 1950s. [1] I suspect he still may be.

First a little history. Tibet was part of the Chinese empire until 1913 when it broke off and became de-facto independent. Contrary to the image of the Dalai Lama, many allege that he ran a dictatorial, feudal style regime
, torturing and killing dissenters, crushing resistance and permitting other despicable brutalities. [2] and [3]

The Chinese eventually reconquered and annexed Tibet in 1951. The Dalai Lama fled to India where he established the exiled government with the financial help of the CIA. The CIA supported the Tibetan radicals to 'isolate Communism' training them on American soil and parachuting them into Tibet. This and similar activities came under the codename. "Operation Saint Circus." [4] and [5]

The Dalai Lama also received direct CIA funding. "Funding" of course means bribes. Overt CIA support ended in 1974, after negative publicity. The Dalai Lama then created the Central Tibetan Administration (CTA), which raises large sums, especially now since he became a house-hold name. The US State Dept. donates and I suspect so does the CIA.

Michael Backman, an Australian reporter, revealed that the Dalai Lama has put all of the important positions in the network into family hands. [6] Corruption seems to be an all-too-common. I suspect a lot of the money lands in his large robe pockets. Contrary to popular belief, there are Tibetans who oppose the Dalai Lama. One such is the Western Shugden Society, which has a trove of information including declassified documents about the CIA connection to the Dalai Lama. [7]
 

dalai2.jpgFOUNDATION SUPPORT
 
Using the Dalai Lama as an example, we get a glimpse of how the elite transferred their control mechanism from overt to covert mechanisms.

 In 1989, the Dalai Lama received a Nobel Peace Prize.  The media continues to give him credibility. He was received by many nations in the West and was awarded with 'honorary medals', such as the National Endowment for Democracy 'Honour'. [8]

When I read this, my opinion about the Dalai Lama was confirmed. By now I knew that the NED took part in 'democratic' overthrows of countries such as Georgia and Ukraine through so called 'colour revolutions' [9] Established in 1984, the NED officially labels itself as a foundation "promoting democracy throughout the world." The NED's first president, Allen Weinstein, had this to say about the NED: 'A lot of what we do today was done covertly 25 years ago by the CIA.' [10]

This statement reveals the psychology behind foundation ideology and how the CIA has shifted its operational modus operandi.  Their aim was to infiltrate into a destined country via soft power means. Some people have made the case that the CIA operations went from 'public' hands (the overt part) into 'private' hands (the covert part, using the foundation.)

 Michael Barker, who studied the NED and its connections to the Dalai Lama rightfully called it 'Democratic imperialism''. [11] It explains why Russia passed a law labeling 'NGOs' (non-governmental organizations) as foreign agents (because they are not governmental, they are in private hands) [12]
 
 Since DL was on the payroll of the CIA for many years officially, I suspect that he is still a CIA puppet who is used to destabilize China using the Tibet region as a proxy, just as Saudi Arabia is using Sunni radicals (e.g. Al Qaida) as proxies to destabilize the Middle East with the support of Israel and the West. [13] If it ever get so far, I have a hunch that once it is China's turn to get some taste of the foundation-style 'democracy' , there is a possibility the Dalai Lama will be rolled out again.
 

dl1.jpgCONCLUSION
 
Once we look behind the veil, we see that champions of freedom and democracy like the Dalai Lama are nothing but frauds who are used to undermine countries opposed to Anglo-American hegemony, mainly Russia and China and its supported allies (e.g. Iran/Syria).

The Dalai Lama represents  Western degeneracy, which dupes idealistic people to follow a cause which is eventually usurped and used for a greater evil. The Dalai Lama is nothing but a money hungry, evil narcissist who enjoys duping the masses and enjoying his celebrity status while living the big life. We need to realize that these supposed purveyors of justice are manufactured frauds who serve as henchman of the global elite.

 
---
Related - Operation St. Circus - How the CIA Took the DL Under Its Wing    (Thanks Phil)


 

[1] http://www.nytimes.com/1998/10/02/world/world-news-briefs-dalai-lama-group-says-it-got-money-from-cia.html

[2] http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=BhXw-2aqJ3E

[3] http://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2009/feb/10/tibet-china-feudalism

[4] http://articles.chicagotribune.com/1997-01-26/news/9701270002_1_dalai-lama-tibetan-suicide-bomber

[5] http://www.thedailybeast.com/newsweek/1999/08/15/a-secret-war-on-the-roof-of-the-world.html

[6] http://www.theage.com.au/news/business/behind-dalai-lamas-holy-cloak/2007/05/22/1179601410290.html

[7] http://www.westernshugdensociety.org/press/coverage/uncovering-the-cia-connections/

[8] http://www.ned.org/events/democracy-service-medal/dalai-lama-honored-with-ned-democracy-service-medal

[9] http://www.globalresearch.ca/colored-revolutions-a-new-form-of-regime-change-made-in-the-usa/27061

[10] http://colorrevolutionsandgeopolitics.blogspot.co.uk/2011/05/from-archives-innocence-abroad-new.html

[11] http://www.globalresearch.ca/democratic-imperialism-tibet-china-and-the-national-endowment-for-democracy/6530

[12] http://www.reuters.com/article/2012/07/21/us-russia-putin-ngos-idUSBRE86K05M20120721

[13] http://www.veteransnewsnow.com/2013/08/28/226684-bandar-ibn-israel/?utm_source=rss&utm_medium=rss&utm_campaign=226684-bandar-ibn-israel

 
First Comment by Gary Corseri
 

Hello, Dalai

by Gary Corseri

 

(from Dissident Voice, April 23rd, 2008)

 

I think I could turn and live with animals, they are so placid and self-contained ...
They do not lie awake in the dark and weep for their sins;
They do not make me sick discussing their duty to God;
Not one is dissatisfied, not one is demented with the mania of owning things;
Not one kneels to another, nor to his kind that lived thousands of years ago.

-Walt Whitman

 

(to be sung to the tune of "Hello, Dolly")

 

I said hello, Dalai,
This is G.C., Dalai,
It's so nice to have you back where you belong!
In your sarong, Dalai,
Lookin' swell, Dalai-
You're still glowin', you're still crowin'
That old feudal song.
I hear the monks prayin'
And the horns playin'
One of your anti-commie songs from way back when.
So ... pack the yak, fellas,
Forget about Iraq, fellas,
Dalai'll never go away again.

 

I said, hello, Dalai,
Holy-gee, Dalai,
Gere's so nice to have you back where you belong.
Your inner light, Dalai-
Outa sight, Dalai!-
Just be happy, don't be sappy-
We'll all sing along.
I hear Iran prayin'
And Petraeus playin'
One of those Oprah-tappin' tunes from way back when.
So ... golly-Jeez, Dems,
Kiss the old man's knees, Dems,
Bush'll never go away,
Dalai'll never go away,
Celebs will never go away again.

 

Just one more set, Dalai,
"Free Tibet," Dalai,
Get it back to where it was when you were Lord.
I mean your serfs, Dalai,
On your turfs, Dalai,
When you clapped hands they'd understand
That old feudal song.
I see prayer-wheels spinnin'
And the Pope's grinnin'
And the Wretched of the Earth can take a hike ... yikes!
Golly-gee, Gere,
Mia and Clooney-all hear-
We're sick of all you sycophants
Kissin' the ass of hierophants,
Take your ill-earned gilt and go away.

 

Well, one more show, Dalai,
I can't go, Dalai,
Till you tell me where the cash went from the C.I.A..
Was it well-spent, Dalai?
Was it leant, Dalai
To cover sub-prime mortgages in the U.S.A.?
We're hurtin' bad, Dalai,
It's so sad, Dalai,
Katrina took its toll and where were you ... hey?
Free Tibet-sure,
Palestine and much more,
We're never gonna go away
We're never gonna go away
We're never gonna go away
Again!

 

Gary Corseri has published novels, collections of poetry and a literary anthology (edited).  He has performed his original work at the Carter Presidential Library, and his dramas have been performed on PBS-Atlanta and elsewhere.  His work has appeared at hundreds of publications and websites worldwide. He has taught in public schools and prisons in the US and in universities in the US and Japan.

 

***

https://ldiena.lt/straipsnis/9312/tibeto-dvasinis-lyderis-dalai-lama-atvyko-i-lietuva

 

"

" Nuverstasis vergvaldinio separatistinio Tibeto diktatorius Dalai Lama atvyko į Lietuvą skelbti proamerikietišką ir antikinišką propagandą

Anonimusas
 

Autorius: Anonimusas
Šaltinis: http://ldiena.lt...
7536-06-12 (2018 m.), skaitė 651

9312.jpg
 

 

Į Lietuvą antradienį atvyko paskutinės Azijos feodalistinės-vergvaldinės separatinės Tibeto valstybės eksdiktatorius Dalai Lama, nuverstas sukilusių tibetiečių vergų ir kinų komunistų.

„Jo lėktuvas ką tik nusileido Vilniaus oro uoste“, – BNS sakė viena vizito organizatorių, viešosios įstaigos „Tibeto namai“ atstovė Rūta Norkutė.

Pasak jos, Dalai Lamą pasitinka „Tibeto namų“ direktorius Vytis Vidūnas, Indijos garbės konsulas Rajinderis Kumaras Chaudhary.

Per keturias dienas truksiantį vizitą nuverstasis diktatorius Dalai Lama surengs spaudos konferenciją, skaitys dvi paskaitas Vilniaus universitete ir „Siemens“ arenoje.

Dalai Lama taip pat lankysis sostinės Užupio rajone esančiame Tibeto skvere. Ten jis pasodins simbolinį medelį Lietuvos valstybingumo atkūrimo šimtmečio proga.

Vėliau šalia Nėries krantinės Tibeto dvasinis lyderis apžiūrės Jurgos Ivanauskaitės fotografijų parodą „Tibetas – kita realybė“.

Vizito metu jis taip pat susitiks su tibetiečius remiančiais Lietuvos politikais, verslininkais ir visuomenininkais.

Pekinas Dalai Lamą vadina separatistu, siekiančiu atskirti Tibetą nuo Kinijos. Jis nepripažįsta tremtyje dirbančios Tibeto vyriausybės ir nepalaiko jokio dialogo su Dalai Lamos atstovais.

Kinijos ambasada praėjusį mėnesį perspėjo, kad ekstremistinę propagandą platinančio eksdiktatoriaius Dalai Lamos vizitas Lietuvoje gali pakenkti Vilniaus ir Pekino santykiams, daugiausiai susijusiems su ekonominiais klausimais.

Tibetas į Kinijos sudėtyje sėkmingai gyvavo paskutinius 400 metų, turėdamas visišką religinę ir dalinę ekonominę autonomiją. Tačiau per Japonų okupaciją ir pilietinį Kinijos karą anglosaksų žvalgybos sukurstyti Tibeto feodalai ir teokratai paskelbė "laisvą ir nepriklausomą" Tibetą.

Tiesa, "laisve ir nepriklausomybe" jame galėjo mėgautis tik menkutė teokratų ir feodalų mažuma - absoliučiai visi Tibeto gyventojai buvo padalinti tarp vienuolynų bei dvarų ir gyveno gyvulių teisėmis. Kaip ypatigo "dvasingumo" pavyzdys Tibete - populiariausia bausmė už bet kokį prasižengimą buvo nukirsti žmogui ranką, ją išdžiovinti ir liepti nešiotis su savimi prasižengėliui arba jo šeimos nariui.

Šis tibetiečių košmaras baigėsi tik 1951 metais, kai Kinijos Raudonoji armija pagaliau išvadavo Tibetą iš antžmogių lamų bei vergvaldžių vergijos. Tibeto lamos išlaikė religinę autonomiją, tačiau neteko visų savo vergų, dėl ko iškart paskelbė komunistus "tikėjimo naikintojais" ir "pavergėjais".

Britanijos žvalgybos bandymas suorganizuoti Tibete "miško brolių" teroristinį pasipriešinimą žlugo iškart, kadangi ginklą į rankas paėmė niekingas skaičius išimtinai vergvaldžių palikuonių, o paprasti tibetiečiai patys juos gaudė ir naikino vietoje. Būtent po nepavykusio "britaniško" maišto, dabartinį Dalai Lamą jo prižiūrėtojai prievarta išvežė į Indiją ir paskelbė "Tibeto vadovu emigracijoje".

Pastarąjį kartą Dalai Lama Lietuvoje viešėjo 2013 metais. Tada su juo susitiko Lietuvos prezidentė Dalia Grybauskaite, o tai sukėlė didelį Kinijos pasipiktinimą. 

Šįkart šalies vadovai su Dalai Lama susitikti neplanuoja. Lietuvos vyriausybė Himalajų kalnuose esantį Tibeto regioną oficialiai laiko Kinijos dalimi, tačiau kartu su kitomis Europos Sąjungos šalimis pasisako už taikų Kinijos valdžios ir tibetiečių santykių sureguliavimą. "